The production and
distribution of scientific information has, of late, frequently become a broken
process. Richard Horton, editor of the pre-eminent medical journal The
Lancet, recently observed, “The case against science is
straightforward: much of the scientific literature, perhaps half, may simply be
untrue. Afflicted by studies with small sample sizes, tiny effects, invalid
exploratory analyses, and flagrant conflicts of interest, together with an
obsession for pursuing fashionable trends of dubious importance, science has
taken a turn towards darkness.”
This is a problem in all
of science. When it comes to industry-sponsered science, industry employs well
thought out and highly researched processes for inducing scientists, doctors,
and the general public to buy into highly biased ideas that are good for the
industry's bottom line. This of course is true for the pharmaceutical industry
just as it is with oil companies and the like.
Not to mention the worst
and by far the most dangerous-to-your-health offender of all, the managed care health insurance industry. George Dawson does an superb job tearing
them a new one on his blog. But this post is about the pharmaceutical
industry, other scientists who do drug studies, and the politics of acceptance
of studies by medical journals.
A well-designed study -
manipulation of study design is a 'hole 'nother issue -
that tries to measure whether a drug is effective for treating certain symptoms
or a certain condition can turn out, broadly, in two possible ways. The drug is
either shown to be effective for the symptom or condition, or it is shown to
not be, or to be not very.
Any single study's result may be invalid due to a problem with the sample of subjects picked, which may be unrepresentative of the whole population of subjects exhibiting the particular condition or symptom under study. It can also be just a coincidence, what with the standard 5% chance of that being so. So in general positive results need to be replicated in several different studies before the US Food and Drug Adminstration (FDA) will approve the drug for public consumption.
Any single study's result may be invalid due to a problem with the sample of subjects picked, which may be unrepresentative of the whole population of subjects exhibiting the particular condition or symptom under study. It can also be just a coincidence, what with the standard 5% chance of that being so. So in general positive results need to be replicated in several different studies before the US Food and Drug Adminstration (FDA) will approve the drug for public consumption.
But what happens if a
drug company or academic scientists who are fighting to get their studies
published so they can attain tenure submit for publication only the
studies that seem to show that a drug works, and then does not submit the other
studies that had a negative result? Well, of course then it looks to everyone
like the evidence for the drug's efficacy is much stronger than it actually is.
It is also true that,
even when a negative study is submitted for publication, the editors of
journals often reject the manuscript. Editors seem to think that, when a study
did not show a positive result, there is no reason to publish it, supposedly
because it does not tell us anything. That's ridiculous, of course - negative
studies can tell us what is not true, and are essential to
good science.
This bias against
so-called "negative" studies happens all over science, by the way,
whether studies are industry sponsored or not. I remember renowned biologist
Stephen Jay Gould decrying this in a book from decades ago.
In order to deal with
the problem of negative studies not seeing the light of day as it applies to
drug studies, a 2007 U.S. Federal law required study authors to report the
results of all of their clinical trials to a public website.
The website is clinicaltrials.gov, which draws 57,000 visitors a
day, including people who are confronting serious diseases and looking for
experimental treatments.
The law was enacted also
because of public concern that a failure to report negative results could harm
participants in similar studies by failing to warn them of possible risks.
The Food and Drug
Administration Amendments Act requires sponsors of most clinical trials to
register and report their basic summary results within 1 year of either
completing data collection for the primary outcome or of terminating. Failure
to report study findings is supposedly punishable by sanctions including civil
penalties of up to $10,000 per day and loss of funding.
So how are we doing? Not
so good.
According to a widely
reported story, a study from Duke University finds that
five years after the reporting law took effect, only 13 percent of
scientists running clinical trials had reported their results! The article
about this study was published online in the New England Journal of Medicine.
Only 13.4% of
investigators reported their results within 1 year, and only 38.3% reported
their results at any time during the study period (N. Engl. J. Med. 2015;372:1031-9). Moreover,
“despite ethical mandates, statutory obligations, and considerable societal
pressure, most trials that were funded by the National Institutes of Health
(NIH) or other government or academic institutions ... have yet to report
results at ClinicalTrials.gov, whereas the medical-products industry has been
more responsive to the legal mandate,” the researchers explained.
Interesting that the
pharmaceutical industry is doing somewhat (although not a whole lot) better than
the NIH-funded scientists on this score.
At 1 year, the rate of
reporting was 17.0% for industry-sponsored trials, 8.1% for NIH-funded trials,
and 5.7% for other government- or academically funded trials. The corresponding
rates of reporting at 5 years were only slightly better, at 41.5%, 38.9%, and
27.7%, respectively.
According to Clinical
Psychiatry News, despite the regulation’s threat of penalties, no
enforcement has yet occurred, the researchers noted, in part because this
portion of the FDA Administration and Amendments Act is still under public
discussion and hasn’t been finalized.
Anyone who wants to
contribute towards changing this situation can do so at Alltrials.net.
The lack of publication of negative studies is not the only strategy employed
by big Pharma to bias everyone's impression of their drug's effectiveness study data. Some other tricks they
employ include:
- Publishing positive studies more than once by using
journal "supplements."
- Conducting a study at multiple locations and then
publishing the results of the individual locations as if they were
separate trials - and doing so selectively if that makes the drug look
better.
- Publishing different measures of drug efficacy at
differnt times to give the impression that the results published later are
from a new or different study.
- Following study patients for longer and longer time
periods and then publishing the results from each time period separately,
again making it look like there was more than one study.
- Publishing positive results in major or more
prestigious journals and negative or neutral studies in more obscure
journals.
- Combining the results of multiple trials in ways that
are more favorable than any individual study in its own right.
Let the buyer beware!
No comments:
Post a Comment