There
has been an increasing debate about whether doctors are using
"evidence-based medicine" or instead are merely going by conventional
wisdom or listening to pharmaceutical company sales pitches. Often, as I have
discussed several times, the so-called "evidence base" consists of
randomized controlled studies (RCT's), while clinical experience is written off
as "anecdotal."
An
anecdote is a report of a single, or at most a few, specific incidents, such as
a patients' seeming to get better after taking a medication, along with the interpretation of the event by an allegedly
biased observer. Besides the fact that one or two cases may not be
representative of a psychiatric condition, and that other causes for the
observed effect have not been ruled out, the description provided by the source
of the anecdote may be incomplete or incorrect. And his or her conclusion may
be logical, or it may be fallacious in any of a variety of different ways.
Widespread
clinical experience — or the clinical experiences of a wide variety of
practitioners who are known to do careful diagnostic work-ups and to follow
patients closely — is somehow also written off as "anecdotal," but
this is just nonsense.
First of all, as I discussed in a previous post,
in psychiatry there are almost no objective diagnostic blood tests or direct
measurements of brain function that can tease out the difference between
neuropathology and normal neural plasticity in response to environmental factors.
Therefore, conclusions drawn in RCT's are based entirely on the self-report
data from subjects or by the potentially biased observations of the
experimenters. They are just as much anecdotal as widespread clinical
experience in that sense.
Actually,
widespread clinical experience is better than most RCT's in determining the
efficacy of drugs. It employs a sample size far larger than all the RCT's on a
treatment put together. Also, there are several important limitations
with RCT's.
In
a paper by G. Parker and S. McCraw (Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavia, 2015:
1-10) about the drug Lamictal (lamotragine), they discuss the disconnect
between the reported efficacy of a new psychotropic medication as quantified in
RCTs and its actual effectiveness as observed in the 'real world' of
psychiatric practice:
"Most commonly any
such disconnect is one of degree, with the medication being progressively
clinically judged as less or more effective. Less commonly, the medication may
be judged to have a different therapeutic 'signal' than for its formal
indication. Two examples of the latter are the selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitors (SSRI's) which seemingly modulate emotional dysregulation as much as
they have antidepressant propensities, and some atypical antipsychotic drugs
having utility in augmenting antidepressant medications and in having mood
stabilizing propensities rather than being confined to the management of
psychotic conditions.
Any such disconnect can
reflect multiple factors, as considered now in relation to antidepressant
medications. An antidepressant's 'efficacy' is evaluated from RCTs with the
'control' treatment being either a placebo or a comparator drug. In such
trials, the duration is often limited to several weeks, the sample constrained by
inclusion and exclusion criteria (e.g. non-suicidal, no substantive co-morbid
conditions) that do not hold in clinical practice, and which may, depending on
recruitment strategies, be weighted to those with potentially spontaneously
remitting and/or less severe conditions.
RCT-evaluated efficacy
may he overestimated using a lower-than-usual dose of any comparator
medication, or underestimated if the antidepressant is prescribed at what might
be later determined to be a suboptimal dose. Biases may emerge if the
sponsoring developers provide data only on trials generating superior findings.
"
Richard
Horton, editor of the medical journal The
Lancet, recently observed, “The case against science is straightforward:
much of the scientific literature, perhaps half, may simply be untrue.
Afflicted by studies with small sample sizes, tiny effects, invalid exploratory
analyses, and flagrant conflicts of interest, together with an obsession for
pursuing fashionable trends of dubious importance, science has taken a turn
towards darkness.”
In
studies used to evaluate medications for treatment of various mood disorders,
there is another glaring problem: the use of inadequate and incomplete
diagnostic evaluations of the subjects. I have written several posts about the
nonsensical expansion of the diagnosis of bipolar disorder, in which
researchers lump together Bipolar I with the bogus disorder Bipolar II.
Readers
of this blog know my attitude about the latter: In my practice since I started
training, I have seen four patients who actually met the DSM criteria for this
supposed disorder when they were interviewed carefully and followed closely,
and all four responded to lithium, suggesting that they were just milder cases
of Bipolar I. And most patients who were given that diagnosis by previous
psychiatrists did not have bipolar disorder at all but had the mood instability
characteristic of certain personality disorders. Some studies have shown this
to be true.
Worse
yet, some studies include patients diagnosed with unspecified bipolar disorder
and "bipolar spectrum disorder." Neither is defined clearly nor
is there any agreement on exactly with what criteria these diagnoses are to be
made. So the RCTS's are basically comparing apples to oranges.
This
particular problem is not discussed clearly in the Parker and McCraw article.
In fact, they conclude that the anticonvulsant drug Lamictal (lamotrigine) is
probably not effective in bipolar I disorder, but probably is effective in
bipolar II disorder - which probably means it may help the affective
instability of patients with personality disorders. This puts this drug as a
possible alternative to SSRI's, which the authors themselves note "... seemingly modulate
emotional dysregulation." Emotional dysregulation and affect or mood
instability are basically the same thing.
(Combining
SSRI's and a long acting benzodiazepine such as clonazepam is even more
effective in "raising the bar" on the strength of environmental
stimuli required to set off an episode of affect dysregulation, as well as in
decreasing self-injurious behaviors like cutting. There are no RCT's on this
combination for these symptoms; the drug companies won't do them because most
of these drugs are generic and the drug companies probably know that it is effective but do not want docs to know this. However, widespread clinical experience by those doctors
who know the difference between bipolar disorder and borderline personality
disorder backs me up on this).
The
history of the FDA approval for the drug Lamictal for bipolar disorder is
bizarre. Its manufacturer first touted it as a treatment for the depressive
episodes in bipolar disorder, although later studies showed it to be
ineffective in the acute phase of bipolar depression. Then, when it got the FDA
approval for psychiatric use, rather than being given the indication for
prophylaxis for (prevention of) episodes of bipolar depression, it was given a
general indication for bipolar disorder as a whole. This, even though there was zero evidence that it prevented episodes of
mania, or that it was useful in acute mania.
In
fact, most of the studies in bipolar disorder with the drug were based on a
rather flawed outcome measure. The drug was found to delay, but not to prevent,
the emergence of another bipolar episode. Whether the episode was a depressive
episode or a manic episode was not specified in the majority of these studies!
Furthermore, lithium — the standard prophylactic treatment—when
used at the dosages which lead to the correct blood levels of the drug in
patients who respond to and can tolerate it (about 80% of bipolar I patients)
completely prevents the re-emergence of manic episodes. In those cases, the
measurement "time until the next manic episode" would essentially be
the patient's entire lifetime.
Delaying episodes of bipolar disorder is better than having them
more frequently, but why would you use a drug to do that when there is another
drug that can prevent them from happening completely?
No comments:
Post a Comment