Family Dysfunction and Mental Health Blog has been selected by Feedspot's panelist as one of the Top 100 Psychology Blogs on the web.
https://psychology.feedspot.com/psychology_blogs/
This blog covers mental health, drugs and psychotherapy with an emphasis on the role of family dysfunction in behavioral problems. It discusses how family systems issues have been denigrated in psychiatry in favor of a disease model for everything by a combination of greedy pharmaceutical and managed care insurance companies, naïve and corrupt experts, twisted science, and desperate parents who want to believe that their children have a brain disease to avoid an overwhelming sense of guilt.
Family Dysfunction and Mental Health Blog has been selected by Feedspot's panelist as one of the Top 100 Psychology Blogs on the web.
https://psychology.feedspot.com/psychology_blogs/
Romanian children who had been stuck in orphanages in which they had almost no direct interactions with their caretakers had been studied extensively by a group of researchers, as described in the book Romania’s Abandoned Children: Deprivation, Brain Development, and the Struggle for Recovery by Charles A. Nelson, Charles H. Zeanah, and Nathan Fox.
Generally, the longer these children were in the orphanages, and the earlier they got there, the more damage to their cognitive abilities, social skills, and stress tolerance there was later in childhood after they were adopted by foster families. Some of them even showed reduced circumferences of their skulls. Hard to imagine that was not permanent damage. Most affected children did seem to show some recovery in their development after adoption, but did not seem to get back completely to normal.
They were not followed much past adolescence in the original study. However, according to a follow-up study described by the BBC, many of these young children adopted by UK families in the early 90s are still experiencing mental health problems even in adulthood (https://www.bbc.com/news/health-39055704).
Despite being brought up by caring new families, this newer long-term study of 165 Romanian orphans found emotional and social problems were commonplace. Initially, all 165 had struggled with developmental delays and malnourishment. While many who spent less than six months in an institution showed remarkable signs of recovery by the age of five or six, children who had spent longer periods in orphanages had far higher rates of social, emotional and cognitive problems during their lives.
Common issues included difficulty engaging with other people, forming
relationships, and problems with concentration and attention levels which
continued into adulthood. This group was also three to four times more likely
to experience emotional problems as adults, with more than 40% having had contact
with mental health services. Despite their low IQs returning to normal levels
over time, they had higher rates of unemployment than other adopted children
from the UK and Romania.
Interestingly, one in five of them seemed to have been unaffected by the neglect they experienced. A small
percentage, to be sure, but if the types of experiences these children had
cause permanent brain damage, how is this even possible?
In current literature
about the effects on the brain of trauma, the common opinion expressed by
experts is that they are irreversible. Brain changes that are seen in various
neurological evaluative procedures - such as different brain scans - do not seem to
go away.
I, on the other hand,
have theorized that the effects of trauma on the brain may be reinforced by
continued interactions with primary attachment figures, whether they be natural
or adoptive parents. If this is going on, the brain changes would not go away. Neural plastic changes leading to this would not happen.
But, people may say, many
of these traumatized Romanian orphans were adopted into loving families! If
what I believe may be true actually is true, why do scars remain in 80% of these
orphans?
Answering this question
definitively is extremely difficult to do. Surely, due to variations in the
genetics of the orphans, some may have been more vulnerable to permanent brain
damage than others. I mean, smaller skulls? But again, on average, and not in
all of them. Also, the experiences of the orphans in the orphanage probably
varied significantly, with a few of them possibly exposed to more helpful
caretakers than the others.
What is rarely discussed
is how difficult it is to raise children who have been traumatized in the way
the orphans had been. They were difficult children with huge behavior
problems, and how to provide proper boundaries in a loving manner is something
that many parents of very normal children have difficulty doing. No matter how
well intentioned the parents may be. Some of these parents might get help from knowledgeable
family therapists, but most do not. So in some ways, the children may be
continuously further traumatized by angry and frustrated parents who don’t have
a clue as to how to best interact with them.
In the original study, there was some
preliminary evidence that interventions by therapists with the adoptive parents
were helpful, but such interventions did not usually take place. Even when they did,
the quality of therapy the parents receive could vary widely.
So addressing the family
dynamics of these adoptive families of adult survivors of the Romanian
orphanage, so that these behavioral and cognitive deficits are no longer being
triggered and reinforced, might possibly lead in many cases to a reversal of both the psychological
deficits as well as the brain changes caused by the original trauma.
I like to think so.
There are currently hundreds of different “schools” of psychotherapy, each with their own theories to account for problematic behavioral, relationships, and thought patterns in individuals. Most of them are variations on the six major schools of thought in the field: psychodynamic, cognitive, behavioral, affect-focused, existential, and family systems. Still, they often have completely different ideas about what is important to focus on in psychotherapy, as well as the reasons for their clients' problems.
In 1985, the Milton Erikson Foundation put on the first of several "Evolution of Psychotherapy" Conferences, in which they were somehow able to get all the current head honchos of the various schools (pictured above). I was there and it was impressive to hear them present their ideas and argue with one another. Before I even became an academic and while in private practice, I had done extensive reading and noticed that each of these differing, very complex schools of thought had valuable things to say about human nature, but that each was riddled with some logical fallacies as well as outright distortions. I decided to attempt to write a book on what I called a "unified theory," which I somehow managed to get published in 1988.
So why so many schools of thought? In a way, this plethora of theories and methodology is not at all surprising in light of the fact that psychology is still a relatively young science, and having several theories is typical of new scientific endeavors. In the case of psychology, coming together is particularly difficult because of the sheer number and magnitude of natural processes involved, coupled with the fact that we cannot read minds. When it comes to important phenomena such as domestic violence and child abuse, people lie all the time – not only to others but to themselves as well. They do so out of shame or a desire to protect other family members.
So-called “empirical” studies in the field are, in a sense, collections of anecdotes: the impression of the researcher coupled with the self report of the individuals being studied. To really know with any certainty what is going on with, say, family interactions , experimenters would have to be able to watch them, over a significant period of time when people were not aware they are being watched. This cannot be done to the extent necessary.
The problems in the field are further made difficult to sort out due to the complex structure of the human brain as well as the sheer number of environmental factors which impinge on it. The brain has billions of neurons, each with up to about 1000 constantly changing synaptic connections caused by a process called neural plasticity. These connections are further impacted by scores of different genes, which do not determine human behaviors but each making certain behavioral tendencies a little stronger or a little weaker.
Relevant environmental influences probably number in the hundreds and come and go in various and constantly changing combinations and intensities. Then there is the so-called “butterfly effect” in which even small differences in initial conditions lead to major differences later on.
Finally, the complexity of the problems that bring clients to therapy varies widely depending on their specific issues. Some problems are rather straightforward like simple phobias or lack of assertiveness with strangers. Others involved horrendous issues such as a family violence or substance abuse and their ongoing effects. One-size-fits-all interventions such as cataloguing irrational cognitions do not really seem adequate for comprehensive treatment.
Treatment outcome studies have been little help. Generally, all the major treatments come out about the same in terms of efficacy. People in SEPI, a professional group that looked at these issues, used to jokingly refer to this as the “Dodo Bird” (from Alice in Wonderland) verdict: All have won and all must have prizes. Even then, a significant percentage of subjects do not respond that well, and those that do improve often the improvement does not last for more than a year. When one paradigm is directly compared with another in a study for a given condition, 85% of the time the treatment favored by the person designing the experiment "wins" and outperforms the other treatment. This is most likely to something called the “allegiance effect.” Another issue: sometimes acceptance of an idea in the field is due to the eminence of the experimenter and not due to the actual evidence.
It seems to me that many of
these schools of thought assume without real evidence that all the problems of
people who are repeatedly self-defeating or self destructive, or who make
choices in life that make them unhappy, do so because they are mentally deficient
in some way. I have categorized these alleged deficiencies as their being either “mad, bad, or
stupid.” That is, insane, evil, or unintelligent. Non-psychotic clients are usually are none of
these things.
Furthermore, as described in a previous post, psychological problems are often seen by
practitioners and theoreticians alike as existing only in people’s heads, as if
the client’s current social and relationship environment is almost irrelevant. For example, in studies of the alleged
over-reactiveness of people diagnosed with so-called borderline personality
disorder, subjects keep diaries of when they have strong emotional reactions –
“ecological momentary assessment” – but are not asked to also write down what
it is they are reacting to.
In the 1980’s and 1990’s, family systems schools began
to address this deficiencies – but then they went to the opposite extreme by
viewing clients entirely as pawns of their kin groups with no capacity for
critical thinking and independent decision making.
Psychiatry, in the meantime, has swung back and
forth between, as L. Eisenberg put it back in 1986, brainlessness (Freudian
psychoanalysis, for example) and mindlessness (eugenics in the 1930’s and the
over-estimation of biological psychiatry in the present).
There are a few of use who are still trying to put all these various ideas together in some sort of valid and coherent form. Gaining acceptance by the field for these efforts is an uphill battle.
When addicts say, "I
can quit using any time I want to," people usually laugh at them and
accuse them of being "in denial." I, on the other hand, believe they
are telling the truth. Since they are admitting they don't want to quit, they
aren't denying anything. Of course, the big question is why they don't want to quit. It can't be because the substance is
making them feel good. They are generally some of the most miserable and unhappy
people around.
So maybe they have some genetic defect that causes a “disease” that impairs their self control? While it is true that genetic tendencies can make someone a bit more or less likely to engage in certain behaviors, the majority of these effects are relatively small.
If genes were causing the problem, one wonders how 12 Step Programs,
which are based on religious conversion techniques that demonize people’s ability
to make good choices for themselves and ask people to surrender to group norms
and do what they are told, would ever succeed. Last I heard, changing your
religion does not lead to major changes in one’s genome. Or its physiological
effects.
I suspect the group’s views and
where their ideas come from have something to do with the addict’s problems. Before
they quit, they are proving the group’s opinions about the evils of
willfulness, and after they quit they are proving the group’s belief system once
again, but in a different way.
And is it really true
that they can’t control their urges? Does an alcoholic actively engaged in drinking and
driving usually take a big swig of Jack Daniels just when a cop pulls up in the
lane next to them? Well, I suppose some might, but I suspect that such
individuals are again going out of their way to prove their group’s idea that
their willfulness is creating their problems. But most will keep the bottle
hidden. So I guess they can control their urges even while intoxicated with
their favorite beverage.
And of course, as I have previously pointed out, animal models of alcoholism are generally poor because scientists can’t seem to find any rats that hide the bottles. And the bottles that people hide are almost always found eventually. Usually by spouses and family members. Imagine that.
Another point: Non-academic people in the
addiction discussion also talk a lot about how co-dependency and
enabling spouses and relatives are part of the problem in someone’s addiction
(see Al-Anon), but somehow today’s scientists seem to think these other folks have nothing to
do with the addict's problems. Really?? In what alternate universe?
I find it impressive how
today’s psychology academics seem to go to any extent to avoid looking at
dysfunctional family dynamics as a major cause of behavior that is destructive to
one’s self or others. For another example, look at speculation about the causes
of the recent spike in students committing mass shootings at schools. If the
role of the parents is looked at at all, it seems that their only role is giving
their kids access to weapons and ignoring danger signs, but not in creating the environment
conducive to motivating their children to act that way in the first place.
Now of course having access to assault weapon is a prerequisite for committing these crimes, but what about the literally millions of homes in the United States in which such guns are found? If the presence of guns were the only problem, these shootings would have been going on at the current frequency for the past decades and decades. For a similar reason, even overt child abuse alone cannot explain the shootings, as the vast majority of abused children do no such things. There has to be other things going on in these families. What, for example, are the parents saying to these kids and to each other, and how are they reacting in general to kids who start to have fantasies of violence?
One of the Columbine shooters in the above picture literally collected an arsenal of weapons in his house prior to the act. When interviewed by the press, his mother said she didn't know about it. I wonder how the shooter interpreted the fact that his mother apparently pretended to not even notice the obvious.
No one seems to be asking these essential questions. Sad.
“A good life balances our own self-interests with other people’s needs…Healthy narcissism is where passion and compassion merge, offering a truly exhilarating life.” ~ Craig Malkin
Balance in life. Lately, that seems like an unknown concept in our black-and-white, all-or-none thinking times.
In his book, Rethinking Narcissism, Dr. Malkin
distinguishes healthy versus unhealthy narcissism, the latter being
characterized by the (dictionary) definition of excessive interests
in one’s own importance and abilities. (In fact, as a described in a previous post,
its base [in Narcissistic Personality Disorder] is often a subconscious sense
of inferiority combined with a sense of not being appreciated
by others).
On the other hand, caring for others at one’s
own expense also has healthy and unhealthy versions. I’ve also written about,
using Barbara Oakley’s term, pathological altruism - in which one’s sacrifices not only lead to misery or
deprivation for the giver but also backfire and lead to harms for its objects.
Although it’s a bit of an oversimplification, I also illustrated it with something I called the Mother Teresa Paradox: if she’s right and giving to others is life’s greatest reward, then by not allowing others to give anything to her, she is in effect depriving everyone else of what she herself defines as the best life has to offer.
A common example in our culture is: the
whore/Madonna complex, in which even married folks feel they are evil if they
enjoy sex too much with one another. Especially women. Men at times and in certain social circles have been allowed to
enjoy it with non-spouses, who are nonetheless derided as whores, because of a need
by their group for them to have sins to atone for on Sundays.
I believe, and my Unified Therapy psychotherapy
paradigm is based on this, is that this sort of craziness is a result of the
evolution of individuality out of collectivism over the last three centuries,
as described in the marvelous book Escape from Freedom by Eric
Fromm. Sometimes it’s best (and was especially in the past) for the survival of
our species if under many circumstances we sacrifice ourselves for the tribe.
But that has become increasing less necessary and even counterproductive
as science and technology have taken center stage. Nonetheless, we are still
primed by our genes to do it (due to kin selection), but it is
becoming more and more counterproductive.
Our own family interactions sometimes don’t keep
up with changing environmental contingencies, leading to something called cultural lag, which leaves families confused and conflicted over which
standards to follow in this regard.
This in turn can lead parents to give destructive mixed
messages to their children. We do have the power to use our critical thinking skills to get everything back into a healthy balance, but are often severely
invalidated by our own families whenever we try, leading to a horrible sense of
not knowing who we are or what we are supposed to do any more (called anomie or groundlessness).
In situations in which a whole family is conflicted over some issue, this is often
indicated when people behave compulsively in one extreme way or in the opposite extreme
way, or bounce back and forth between the two
extremes.
Problems like these have to be discussed if they
are to be solved, but people are often too ashamed or defensive to do so. The
countermeasure is empathy, which comes from doing research into one’s family
background in order to understand why our parents are driving us crazy. How to
employ this is described in both my psychotherapy paradigm for
self-destructive behavior (which by definition cannot be selfish unless an
individual is nearly brainless) and in my self-help book for somewhat more functional families.
It was really impressive when my patients had an
“a-ha” moment that led to the reaction of “So THAT’S why they act that way!" It
was very liberating for them, although that freedom can still easily be undone
by aggressively invalidating family attachment figures. I teach strategies for
getting the parents to stop doing that.
If you are in a cycle of self-destructive
behavior, such as, say continually going back to an abusive marriage because
your parents seem to be blaming you for it (and if you have been going back, it is not “blaming
the victim” to say that you bear some responsibility for your own plight), my
message to you is to learn about this stuff and how it has affected you personally and
your family, and to take charge.
Why aren’t many kids seemingly growing up as maturely as they used to any more? Why are mental health problems and suicidal ideation as well as actually suicides increasing? Why are more and more children losing self confidence and feeling defective?
In a new book by Abigail Shrier, Bad Therapy: Why the Kids Aren’t Growing up, the author blames the mental health establishment. So does the parenting guru I’ve been reading for years, John Rosemond. And psychologists are indeed a big part of the problem. But both miss an important aspect of the phenomenon.
The definition of “traumatized” in children has been expanded beyond all recognition by the profession. In the mental health field, consideration of the effects of adverse childhood experiences have gone back and forth from one extreme to the other: the serious ones at times are almost completely ignored. At other times child abuse was thought to be everywhere. And now trauma is seen as almost any occurrence that makes a kid in the least bit unhappy or stressed.
I described what has been going on at the college level in my review of the book, The Coddling of the American Mind, with students' reactions to “microaggressions,” and political incorrectnesss being equated with PTSD caused by a terrifying combat experience.
Nowadays, according to Shrier, kids are
seen as being unable to put aside even hurt feelings in order to concentrate on
the school work in front of them. Resilience is now seen as “accepting” these
“traumas” rather than dealing with them in a potent manner. Personal agency has seemed
to have “snuck out the back door.”
And 40% of the current, rising
generation has received psych treatment versus 26% of gen-X’ers when they were
younger. More and more phony psych diagnoses are put on kids, often at the
suggestion of teachers. More and more children are afraid to be wrong in school
laboratories or to test new ideas for fear of making a mistake. Bullies
are being suspended less and less frequently for fear of damaging their self esteem.
American children are more likely than others to exaggerate all kinds of risks.
For those mental health professionals who do recognize all this as a problem, the usual explanation for why it is happening is that when parents and teachers over-protect and over-pathologize their children, they are preventing them from learning social skills which, it is believed, cannot be “taught” in most cases but must be learned through trial and error.
If a
parent always steps in, or even when parents don’t let their children go out to
play or walk to school because they believe that something bad will happen to
them, the kids are said to never get the chance to learn those things. As the author also
points out, sometimes feeling mildly to moderately anxious or moody can be a
good thing since it can motivate kids to evaluate their situation and lead them to
take action.
Now don’t get me wrong. There is
much truth to these assertions. What’s missing, however, is the way this sort
of treatment by parents and teachers is interpreted by the children themselves. The children start to see
themselves as a big burden to their over-anxious, worrying parents. Not
only that, but the parents seem angry about it. I believe that if a child feels
like too big a burden to their parents, they may start to think their parents
would be better off without them. This could increase their risk of suicide.
Why? Because, as I have been arguing
for years, children are willing to sacrifice their own best interests in order
to stabilize their parents. This is due to the evolutionary force called kin
selection. It is not just that kids don’t experiment with new behavior in
order to figure out how to, say, respond to a bully. Hell, there are TV shows, YouTube channels, and many other sources for suggestions that they could try out at school. But as long as they feel the need to let their parents take
care of them, they are not motivated to become independent. "Enabling" parents
lead to co-dependent children.
Schrier does allude to this aspect of the process involved here, but it is not clear to me that she
truly appreciates the extent of the issue. She does say that kids often feel
responsible for their parents, and may feel like a “constant burden to their
stricken parents.” She also says that there is nothing scarier to them than
parents “overmatched and afraid.” She has also noticed that people who make parenting
look exhausting do not seem all that fond of the kids they raised. If an untrained observer like the author can see this, then guess what? So can the children. And they will be induced
to make any necessary sacrifice.
In my last post about the internal family
systems therapy model, I discussed how some newer models (ones that come close
to my own) seem to completely ignore the ongoing nature of
repetitive dysfunctional family behavior even after the children grow up. Or as
the book It Didn't Start with You by Mark Wolynn sometimes
does, what happens with an adult child’s interactions with parents in the present.
I have championed the idea of the
intergenerational transfer of dysfunctional family “rules” that are due to a
previous group or individual trauma. I am very happy to report that this subject
seems to finally be getting the attention it deserves. In my very first book,
originally released way back in 1988, I proposed very specific
mechanisms through which this occurs.
While there is a genetic component to this such
as changes in the body's stress response reactions (e.g., release of cortisol),
why do we think these have to be permanent? If they were, then treatment or
therapy would do precious little.
Perhaps the reason they do not change is that
they are continually reinforced in the present. Neural plasticity
tells us that brain circuits are strengthened or weakened depending on how much
stimulation they get, and relevant interactions with parents do not stop at age
18, or even at age 3-5 as some analysts used to think.
Especially since the circuits are created and then reinforced (or not
reinforced) by attachment interactions in the first place.
The subtitle of the book under review here is How
Inherited Family Trauma Shapes Who We Are and How to End the Cycle. The
author mentions that in many many cases traumatized parents and grandparents
avoid talking about what happened to them, mostly out of shame. I completely
agree. However, that does not mean that there is nothing that
can be communicated through a variety of other behaviors in ongoing
interactions.
Wolynn
gets ever so close to understanding what’s going on, but is IMO missing the
continuous drama. When he notices that sometimes parents have not discussed the
trauma, he in fact does wonder how then the trauma might be passed down. He
over-emphasizes genetics.
It is true that if a mother were traumatized,
that can affect how she interacts with a kid, which can itself be traumatizing.
This can lead to epigenetic changes (genes being turned off and
on) in the child that affects their reactivity and
perhaps their proneness to certain medical and psychiatric disorders. But
that’s as far as it goes. He mentions that epigenetic changes occur mostly
through a chemical process called methylation, but seems to think they are not
reversible. If a gene that regulates other genes can be methylated, it can be unmethylated.
He
gives an example of a boy named Jessie who at the age of 19 suddenly developed
severe insomnia accompanied by freezing, shivering and an inability to keep
warm. He had no major problems sleeping before this. He had to drop out of
college because of these symptoms. Doctors could find nothing wrong. Jessie
later revealed to Schwartz that he had only recently became aware of the fact
than an uncle he never knew he had froze to death – at the age of 19.
So
does this mean that this was some sort of genetic effect? Even the author
seemed bit skeptical. Let’s face it: genes do not and cannot contain specific
memories like dates when traumas occur. Maybe the father, whose brother it was,
started acting strangely in some way when his son reached that age.
Wolynn
also falls for an aspect of the heritability
fraud when he agrees that all children grow up in the same
family, so this must be a “shared” environment. But somehow he is also
aware of the Murray Bowenesque understanding that parents can relate to
each of their children much differently than to the others for a variety of
reasons. This is especially common in so-called dysfunctional families.
Not
to mention the fact that siblings can all be affected by the family trauma in
very different ways despite the specific nature of the mother’s traumas and any
resultant internal conflicts. And some of children may not affected much
at all. He never addresses the clear contradiction in these
ideas. What distinguishes those who do from those who don’t? Are their
genomes that different?
Another
thing that Wolynn does not seem to be aware of is similar to the lack of
understanding by Richard Schwartz in internal family systems therapy that I
described in my last post. He does not quite seem to get that a lot of the
people he writes about are not protecting themselves, but are
in fact self-destructive. The case of “Elizabeth” on page 205
illustrates this clearly. According to the author, she felt rejected by her
mother and so feared that everyone else was going to reject her. She would then
feel left out and all alone.
But
her response to this? Isolating herself. Left out and all alone. The very thing
she claimed to fear! At her job she almost completely separated herself from
co-workers and would barely talk to anyone all day. She was not described
as being anywhere near stupid enough to not see the rather obvious results of
what she was doing.
In
this case as a therapist, I would ask a modified version of the Adlerian
question to find out who she was sacrificing herself for: If I had
a magic wand and could make you accepted and popular, and prevented you from
screwing that up, who might be negatively affected?
As most of my readers know, I believe in a family systems view of
the way patients who are repetitively self-destructive or self-sabotaging are
acting. I do not think it’s “all in their heads,” but that it serves some sort
of purpose for the members of their families as a whole.
Furthermore, this is ongoing and continues to take place in the
present, not only when they may have been initially traumatized as children.
The family continues to interact over the trauma, because most people continue
to have relationships with their parents. If they don’t seem to, it’s often
because they are communicating through third parties. In this and the following
post, I’m going to illustrate how the field does not take this into account,
even when they look at systems issues.
I have also been writing about how the psychotherapy profession
has seemingly given up on family systems theory, except for some masters level
family therapists, because of a variety of social factors. Some readers may
argue with me about that because of the growing popularity of Richard
Schwartz’s Internal Family Systems Therapy. I really had not read much about it
because I always wondered why he was dealing with internal models of the family
system and not the actual living family members. Sort of like Schema Therapy or
working on one’s “inner child.”
I finally got around to reading a book he wrote for lay readers in
2008 called You Are the One You’ve Been Waiting For. Although I
think the kind of treatment described therein can indeed be helpful in many
situations in which interactions with one’s parents are not too complicated, I
do not think it would have helped most of my patients with severe personality
disorders.
And even in cases in which it does work, IMO it would be even more
effective if it paid attention to some other factors which the author seems
completely unaware of, as illustrated by an extensive case example he wrote
concerning a narcissistic doctor he names “Kevin” and his wife “Helen,” who was
described as a bright woman with a career of her own. I will talk about this
case in a moment.
One
thing about the IFS model that I’m not enamored with is his descriptions of
peoples’ personality “parts” — protectors, controllers, exiles, critics and
tor-mentors. I would like to believe he is speaking metaphorically, but he
seems to be following along with the tradition (in my opinion
inaccurate) of dividing up a personality into little beings running around
inside someone’s head with thoughts, desires, and feelings all their own – not
unlike the animated characters inside a girl’s head in the Inside Out movies.
Just like the psychoanalysts used to refer to id, ego, and superego as if they
were parts of the brain. I find the concept of a “false self” to be much more
useful.
A bigger problem is that he sees the problematic behavior of people as trying to protect themselves from reliving past hurts. He says they scan the environment looking for threats and run away at the slightest hint of one.
That wasn’t at
all what I saw in my patients.
People who couple off with people in a dysfunctional relationship
were not only not looking for red flags, they were actively ignoring them! If
anything, they seem to be attracted to certain dangers, like a
woman from an alcoholic family who marries one alcoholic after another.
I see them as protecting their parents and
the ongoing systems dynamics, not themselves per se. I fail to
see how being self-destructive can be selfish, unless someone is unbelievably
unintelligent. I will give a possible explanation of Kevin’s and Helen’s
behavior in that case, consistent with this idea, so back to that.
According to the author, for thirty years Helen had put up with
Kevin’s “carping about her taste in clothes, her child rearing, her political
opinions, her education, her intelligence, and her logic.” He frequently dissed
her in public. She also was said to hate his long work hours and felt
neglected. And now she suddenly said if he didn’t straighten up and fly right,
she was outta there.
Wait…she put up with this behavior for thirty years before
the ultimatum? Why so long? And how did Kevin interpret her staying despite the
fact that he must have repeatedly heard many of her complaints during
that period. The author attributes Helen’s sudden attack of courage to their
youngest child graduating high school, but many people with kids still at home
get divorced.
Furthermore, the book does not mention any ongoing interactions
during the marriage with parents and in-laws, despite the fact that he
attributes Keven’s behavior to his parent’s behavior towards him when he was a
kid. Did the parents just evaporate? Did Helen possibly get brave because they - or her own parents - died? We don’t know.
According to the book, Kevin’s father left his mother when he was seven, after a fight between them in which the dad hit his mom. The Dad remarried and never saw Kevin again. Mom then went on to have a series of problematic relationships. Sometimes, she seemed to not want to have Kevin around. Kevin handled this by throwing himself into academic and professional success and becoming fiercely independent. I believe this was meant to demonstrate to his mother that he did not “need” her or anyone else.
After some individual and couples counseling with the very
empathic Doctor Schwartz, and when he is starting to get in better touch with
himself, Kevin remembers blaming his mother for his dad’s leaving and making her
cry just after the breakup when he was seven. He says the “little boy” inside
him would rather die than upset her again.” That sounds like, in his mind
at least, he is protecting her - not himself - and feels guilty about what he did.
What I would hypothesize is that the mother indeed felt very guilty about the Dad leaving her and how it affected Kevin, and was blaming herself. This is why she became uncomfortable when he was around and seemed to prefer he be gone and be independent of her “bad” (in her view) influence on him. So he tried to make her feel less guilty by being successful without any need of her. And later maybe displaced the anger he felt towards his mother onto Helen (while displacing his anger at his abandoning father onto his co-workers).
So
perhaps Helen’s “job” was to stabilize him by allowing him to do this displacing. Her
initial attraction to him was probably due to something similar to this dynamic going on in her own family.
When he got better, he was able to be more empathic with his
mother’s behavior because he realized she felt she did not really deserve any
love. I agree that understanding a parent’s problematic behavior helps people
feel better about themselves – and about the parents. But of course this aspect
of her mother’s behavior just kicks the question of the reason for all this
back another generation. Why was she like that?
Schwartz comes quite close to understanding of the dialectical way dysfunctional couples interact, with
each enabling the false self of the other and punishing any evidence of each's own true
self. Each denies that they need this help themselves even as they give it to the
other. They have motives for doing so that have to do with
stabilizing their parents, but they also hate that they have to be like this.
They are ambivalent. So they also complain about it. Mixed messages!
Schwartz gets away from these ideas by instead thinking that "vicious
circles" of interactions are present- just as systems therapists also suppose. But then he
also calls it a dance. In a dance one member of the couple may
be leading, but simultaneously, the other is closely following. If either fails
to do their part, there is no dance.
Once a couple gets in touch with these patterns, however they do
it, this means that there is another issue (apart from ongoing interactions with living parents) that arises that may further greatly complicate making changes. Even if, say, the wife is ambivalent about her role, she has
thought all along that her husband wanted and needed her to perform it. So if he
suddenly says he no longer wants this without acknowledging his ambivalence, at
first she won’t believe him.
But if he somehow convinces her he is sincere, the wife is still prone
to think to herself, “Hey wait. I have been sacrificing for him for all this
time, and only now he tells me that he did not want me to! And
he no longer wants to enable me?” She feels betrayed. This of course also comes
out as a double message as her true self really wants the change too. Schwartz
talks about members of the couple being triggered by the other, but misses this
very important reason why.
Wikimedia Commons: Various Pills, Unknown author, Creative Commons Attribution-Share Alike 3.0
I often write about the
misuse of psychiatric drugs (and disease mongering by big Pharma). Recently,
two journal articles have been published that are in line with what I’ve been
saying. (Caveat: I want to make it clear that I am not against the use of
psychiatric drugs, which can be very useful and effective when prescribed
properly to the right patients).
One of my pet peeves has been the demonization in the psychiatric literature of a class of drugs called benzodiazepines. Probably because they are cheap as well as effective. This includes drugs like Valium, Librium, clonazepam and Xanax. They are used for sleep and anxiety disorders. Whenever they are referred to in the psychiatric press, references to the names are almost always immediately followed by the phrase, “but of course they are addictive.”
In
contrast, references to other drugs, say anti-psychotic meds, are never
accompanied by the words, “but of course they can cause diabetes and chronic
movement disorders." Nor is any such statement attached to references to a
far more often-abused class of drugs: stimulants like Adderall.
Even more strangely, this
statement is also usually NOT applied to the references to the so-called
“Z-drugs” like Ambien and Lunesta (which are newer and more lucrative for
pharma), even though they work in almost exactly the same way as benzos and are
just as addictive.
Benzo’s are highly
effective for the treatment of short-term insomnia and anxiety, and particularly
for the highly disabling panic disorder when it does not respond to an
antidepressant alone. While benzo’s certainly can be abused, most of the time
they are not. They are listed by the FDA as Schedule 4, which means low abuse
potential. Adderall is Schedule 2, meaning a high potential for abuse.
So is benzo addiction
really a big problem, especially now that doctors can see if their patients
have been getting them from more than one provider? (With the exception of the most
addictive benzo – Xanax - there is also no big street market for them). In
general, shorter acting drugs are more addictive than longer acting ones, since
withdrawal symptoms come much more quickly.
A new, huge study in
Denmark has been published that is consistent with my experience (Rosenquist, T.W.
et. al., “Long-Term Use of Benzodiazepines and
Benzodiazepine-Related Drugs: A Register Based Danish Cohort Study.” American
Journal of Psychiatry 181.3, March 2024). It found that
only 15% of users stayed on the drugs for over a year, and only 3% for more
than 7 years. The median dose stayed rather stable in this population. Long
term use of Z drugs was on average higher than with those on a benzo. Patients
escalating their intake to higher than prescribed doses was uncommon and was
found mostly in people that abused other drugs.
An accompanying editorial
in the American Journal of Psychiatry points out that
conditions like dementia, drug abuse, and other chronic illnesses often cause bad outcomes, not the treatment itself. As to overdoses, with benzo's they almost are never
fatal unless the drugs are combined with opiates (on which one can overdose all by themselves).
When it comes to the overuse of antipsychotics, adding them to an antidepressant in “treatment resistant depression” is widely discussed in the psychiatric press - no doubt because many mental health consultants work for Pharmaceutical companies.
Now don’t get me wrong, these medications definitely can work in
this capacity – I had luck adding Abilify to an antidepressant. (As soon as
that drug went generic, the exact same TV ads for depression using a
replacement brand-named drug named Rexulti started. The two drugs are nearly identical [what does
that tell you?)].
The issues are: 1. Using
two drugs when one will suffice, and 2. The potential severe side effects of
anti-psychotics (especially diabetes and a chronic movement disorder called
tardive dyskinesia). There are safer “augmentation” drugs like lithium to try
first.
I’ve discussed in a
previous post one big reason why depression is
labeled "treatment resistant" when it may not be: the conflation of major
depression with chronic unhappiness. The latter almost never responds
to an antidepressant (not counting the times when it acts like a sedative and
is only effective due to that side effect).
Also, you can be
chronically unhappy before a major depressive episode even starts and that is
your baseline. If you end up at your baseline, then the antidepressant did work!
In that case, the next step should be psychotherapy, not more meds.
These issues tie in with an
idea discussed by H. Paul Putman III M.D. in the April 2024 issues of Psychiatric
News. He points out that much of what is labeled “treatment resistance” is
actually a treatment impasse, meaning the doctors have not done everything they needed to do with the antidepressant or ruled out other causes for the
symptoms. They have perhaps not slowly raised the dose until the maximum, or if this becomes precluded by side effects, then trying the same strategy with a second
antidepressant and then a third.
Medical causes such as
endocrine disorders have not been ruled out. Maybe there was a rupture in the
alliance between the patient and the doctor. Or the patient has not been taking
the prescribed antidepressant at full dose all along - or not taking it at all.
Interactions with other medications have not been evaluated. Co-occurring psychiatric disorders
that are complicating treatment have not been considered.
Putman says the term
“difficult to treat” should be substituted for “treatment resistant.” But then
Pharma might not make as much money.