Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) is currently the predominant psychotherapy treatment paradigm taught to clinicians-in-training in psychology graduate schools. However, when I first received psychotherapy training in the mid 1970’s, by far the predominant school of psychotherapy was psychoanalysis. We did receive a smattering of behavior therapy training, and we were even assigned one book about family systems ideas by Virginia Satir. Our training program was a bit unusual in that regard.
Just
like the CBT industry does now, analysts exaggerated the validity of the scientific
evidence for psychoanalytic theory, and made grossly inflated claims about the
effectiveness of psychoanalytic treatment. Its theory was applied to everything, even to schizophrenia, although
by then it was pretty clear to most of us that they were completely wrong about
that condition. I’m surprised analysts did not try to treat ingrown toenails
with psychoanalysis.
Just
as now, economics played a huge part. (That’s why I just called it the
CBT industry). When I made some rather mild criticisms of one aspect of CBT theory and practice on my Psychology
Today blog, a CBT therapist wrote a rebuttal, and several people wrote in to say how unethical I was because
I was not practicing a “scientifically validated” form of psychotherapy, and
was therefore by implication a snake oil salesman.
Readers of my book, How Family Dysfunction Spurs Mental Disorders, know that the “evidence base” in all of psychotherapy is actually quite weak, and that CBT controls the funding of research and denies it to practitioners from other schools – although nonetheless there are still many studies from other schools that are every bit as strong (or should I say as weak) as the CBT studies. That includes analytically-oriented therapies, as a meta-analysis by Jonathan Shedler in the February-March 2010 issue of the American Psychologist clearly showed.
Readers of my book, How Family Dysfunction Spurs Mental Disorders, know that the “evidence base” in all of psychotherapy is actually quite weak, and that CBT controls the funding of research and denies it to practitioners from other schools – although nonetheless there are still many studies from other schools that are every bit as strong (or should I say as weak) as the CBT studies. That includes analytically-oriented therapies, as a meta-analysis by Jonathan Shedler in the February-March 2010 issue of the American Psychologist clearly showed.
Not
to mention that most psychotherapy outcome studies rely on so-called treatment manuals that spell out what
therapists are supposed to do, so that all the therapists in the study are
doing the same thing – regardless of how the patient is reacting to what they
are doing. NO competent practicing clinician does
that.
Psychotherapy involves science of course, but in many ways it is an art form as well. People are complicated, and each patient is unique in many ways, and can always choose to respond positively, negatively, or not at all to any intervention made by a therapist.
Psychotherapy involves science of course, but in many ways it is an art form as well. People are complicated, and each patient is unique in many ways, and can always choose to respond positively, negatively, or not at all to any intervention made by a therapist.
Additionally, each treatment manual is comprised of multiple ingredients – some of which may
help or be the primary active ingredient, some of which may do nothing, and
others of which may actually be counterproductive. And yet many CBT proponents argue as if everything they do has been scientifically
proven.
It
is clear to me that CBT practitioners say this stuff for the same reason that
Big Pharma controls and distorts the practice of doing, and the dissemination
of results from, studies of medication effectiveness: They want to “sell” their
goods and stamp out the competition. (BTW,
that does not mean that therefore a pharma-sponsered study has NO validity -
only that bias must be taken into account).
Analysts
also protected their turf back in the day, very arrogantly, although ultimately they failed. As a
trainee, if you criticized any aspect
of analytic theory, you were told in no uncertain terms that you needed to go
into psychoanalysis yourself, so you could find out why you were “resistant” to
analytic theory. In other words, the
only reason you were questioning the theory was because you were neurotic!
This recommendation involved the use of not one, but three logical fallacies, all wrapped up in a single statement. It was a non sequitur, since someone might be questioning the theory for any number of other reasons besides their own psychological issues. It was an ad hominem attack, since it was going after the questioner and not the question. And of course it was begging the question. The accusation of being neurotic might be true if analytic theory is true, but that is the very issue in doubt and under debate.
This recommendation involved the use of not one, but three logical fallacies, all wrapped up in a single statement. It was a non sequitur, since someone might be questioning the theory for any number of other reasons besides their own psychological issues. It was an ad hominem attack, since it was going after the questioner and not the question. And of course it was begging the question. The accusation of being neurotic might be true if analytic theory is true, but that is the very issue in doubt and under debate.
Another
trick that different economic interests in mental health use to denigrate the
competition also involves the issue that all of the so called schools of
therapy do not consist of single ideas, but consist of a large number and a wide variety
of different ideas and techniques that are tied together by some common threads. Anyone who bothers to think about it has to
know that some of these ideas could be wrong while others could still be right, or
that an idea may have validity for some situations and for some phenomena while being
inappropriate for explaining or addressing others.
For
instance, people in the field may try to argue against all the ideas of a
particular school, even though many are obviously quite valid, by throwing up a
few incidences of when the school had something clearly wrong. They will of course pick the most egregious
examples they can find. To criticize psychoanalysis, they might bring up such discredited - and now fairly much discarded - analytic ideas such as "penis envy," or the wild overemphasis on the Oedipus Complex.
Hyper-biological psychiatrists love to bring up the awful effects of the fallacious psychoanalytic theory of the “schizophrenogenic mother” to argue against all of psychotherapy, not even just psychoanalysis! I tell biological reductionists that if they don’t hold the theory of schizophrenogenic mothers against psychotherapists, then I will not hold the theory of eugenics against them.
Hyper-biological psychiatrists love to bring up the awful effects of the fallacious psychoanalytic theory of the “schizophrenogenic mother” to argue against all of psychotherapy, not even just psychoanalysis! I tell biological reductionists that if they don’t hold the theory of schizophrenogenic mothers against psychotherapists, then I will not hold the theory of eugenics against them.
I
even hear Pharma-sponsored speakers indirectly and implicitly attack therapy by
posing really, really stupid questions such as “What is better for depression –medication
or psychotherapy?” Well, first of all,
that’s like asking, “What’s better for patients with coronary artery disease,
taking nitroglycerin when they feel chest pain, or losing weight?” Those two interventions target two completely
different aspects of the problem.
Second, the question lumps together all types of psychotherapy. What type of therapy are we talking about? Which interventions? It would be like me arguing against the use of medication by pointing out how ineffective penicillin is for treating clinical depression.
Second, the question lumps together all types of psychotherapy. What type of therapy are we talking about? Which interventions? It would be like me arguing against the use of medication by pointing out how ineffective penicillin is for treating clinical depression.
Just as with many aspects of CBT (CBT'ers please take note that I am saying that), many aspects
of psychoanalytic theory retain much explanatory power. They are so widely accepted that they have
even become part of the cultural conventional wisdom in industrialized
countries.
Who doesn’t believe that people sometimes take their anger out about something on someone or something else? Mad at your boss, come home and kick the dog? That’s the defense mechanism of displacement. Yeah, like that never happens.
Who doesn’t believe that people sometimes take their anger out about something on someone or something else? Mad at your boss, come home and kick the dog? That’s the defense mechanism of displacement. Yeah, like that never happens.
Intrapsychic
conflict creating emotional and interpersonal problems because people want something really bad but feel guilty about it? Check. Conversations have unspoken subtexts? Check. Acting towards authority figures in a certain way because they remind
you of your father? That’s transference. Check. CBT folks may prefer to call the phenomenon schemas instead, but it's still transference.
People wanting to avoid unpleasant subjects and in response changing the subject or explaining away inconvenient facts? That's resistance. Check.
Forgetting about unpleasant memories? Well, whether that’s unconscious or subconscious may be debatable, but the fact that repression exists? The whole Catholic Church child molestation scandal started with a case of "recovered" memory. Check.
People wanting to avoid unpleasant subjects and in response changing the subject or explaining away inconvenient facts? That's resistance. Check.
Forgetting about unpleasant memories? Well, whether that’s unconscious or subconscious may be debatable, but the fact that repression exists? The whole Catholic Church child molestation scandal started with a case of "recovered" memory. Check.
The
analysts are even right about personality problems stemming from childhood
experiences within the family. The
attachment literature is extremely powerful, and we all know that one of the
biggest risks for just about every psychiatric condition in the DSM is a
history of childhood abuse and/or neglect.
Ah,
but there is where the psychoanalysts started to go wrong. They seemed to
assume that childhood experiences completely determined
what psychological problems a patient has and that subsequent experiences
were somehow inconsequential. Orthodox
analysts believe that your personality is fixed by the time you are five years
old. Some go back even further than that.
Of
course, if subsequent experiences could not affect personality, it would do a
person no good at all to go into psychoanalysis, because the experience of
psychotherapy would have absolutely no effect - according to the orthodox
analysts’ own assumptions about personality formation.